- Original article
- Open access
- Published:
To behave or not (un)ethically? The meditative effect of mindfulness on statistics anxiety and academic dishonesty moderated by risk aversion
International Journal for Educational Integrity volume 20, Article number: 6 (2024)
Abstract
Despite the growing interest in mindfulness in higher education, the literature on its relation to decision-making under risk (i.e. academic misconduct) and statistics anxiety is scarce. The present research shall fill this gap. Based on the prospect theory, we assessed the mediating effect of mindfulness on the relationship between statistics anxiety and academic dishonesty moderated by risk aversion. Data were collected from 791 undergraduate students in six Israeli academic institutions studying for bachelor’s degrees in social sciences. Questionnaires included the following measures: risk behaviour according to the prospect theory framework, Mindful Attention Awareness Scale, Statistics Anxiety Rating Scale, Academic Misconduct Scale and sociodemographic variables. Correlations among these variables were explored. The data was analysed using Structural Equation Modelling (SEM). The results indicate that the variance in academic dishonesty is explained by students' statistics anxiety with a mediation of Mindfulness moderated by Risk Aversion. Mindfulness negatively affects Academic Dishonesty, while Risk Aversion has a significant positive effect on Mindfulness. Finally, among individuals with high statistics anxiety, Risk Averse individuals show significantly higher Mindfulness than Risk Seekers. We conclude that mindfulness-based interventions might be a constructive tool to reduce risk-taking and promote ethical decision-making among individuals who experience high levels of statistics anxiety. Furthermore, developing mindful skills may help individuals with higher anxiety levels neutralize these unwanted feelings and get along with their learning tasks. Hence, avoid academic unethical behaviours.
Introduction
Transiting into post-secondary education (tertiary, college, or university) is a meaningful milestone for many young adults (Hirshberg et al. 2022). Many manage well, while others experience anxiety, stress, or depression (Parsons et al. 2022) due to academic pressures or poor coping behaviour (Smit & Stavrulaki 2021). Some scholars assert that anxiety is associated with avoidance behaviour (Hasty et al. 2021) and that individuals tend to avoid or reduce their fear of failure (Widhiastuti & Kanaka 2021), which may lead to fraudulent and counterproductive academic behaviours (Cuadrado et al. 2020). Counterproductive academic behaviour, such as academic dishonesty, is a complex phenomenon affecting educational institutions worldwide (Salgado et al. 2022). The research determined that global rates of academic misbehaviour worsened significantly (Eaton 2021).
Higher education is essential as it outlines students’ future professional careers (Eltayeb 2021). Statistical knowledge has been recognised as an important tool in academic education (Steinberger et al. 2021; Trassi et al. 2022). Today, academic training includes compulsory introductory statistics courses. Some students associate these with high anxiety levels (O’Bryant et al. 2021), termed statistics anxiety—a momentary negative emotional state when faced with statistical tasks (Onwuegbuzie et al. 1997). Research indicated that high levels of statistics anxiety impair proper academic performance among students (Trassi et al. 2022), resulting in lower performance rates, procrastination, course drop-out (Kaufmann et al. 2022), or academic dishonesty (Eshet et al. 2022).
Nevertheless, using mindfulness techniques, students might alleviate the negative effects of statistics anxiety. A precursory study demonstrated the usefulness of mindfulness in reducing anxiety in introductory statistics classes (Lesser 2017). Furthermore, mindfulness is also a predictor of individuals' (un)ethical decision-making (Götmann et al. 2021), risk behaviour (Y. Zhang et al. 2021a, b), and academic outcomes (Kuroda et al. 2022). Additionally, mindfulness is negatively associated with performance anxiety (Kuroda et al. 2022) and deviant behaviour (Müller 2021). However, statistics anxiety mediating factors remain underexposed (Trassi et al. 2022).
Despite the growing interest in mindfulness in higher education (Ergas & Hadar 2021), to the best of our knowledge, research literature that links risk behaviour, academic misconduct, statistics anxiety, and mindfulness is scarce. The present research shall fill this gap based on the prospect theory widely used concerning risk behaviour (Prietzel 2020) and academic misconduct (Salgado et al. 2022). This study assessed the mediating effect of mindfulness on the relationship between statistics anxiety and academic dishonesty moderated by risk aversion. Comprehending the relationship between all the aforementioned research variables among undergraduate students allows us to understand this relationship more deeply, personalise academic interventions, and, as a result, reduce academic dishonesty and statistics anxiety.
Theoretical background
Mindfulness
Mindfulness is a traditional Buddhist concept (Dawson 2021), defined as an intentional, nonjudgmental awareness of the present moment. It is a consciousness quality of awareness and a reflexive state of internal and external states or events (Brown and Ryan 2003a) with sustained attention to the current moment (Vaughn et al. 2013). Contemporary and Buddhist theory advocates that mindfulness embraces awareness of various objects (i.e., bodily sensations), mind (i.e., thoughts, emotions), and hedonic experiences (i.e., pleasant or unpleasant experiences) (Hadash et al. 2023). Mindful awareness also involves the flexibility of attention and inhibits interpretation, elaboration, and analysis (Aksen et al. 2023). In other words, mindfulness is awareness of objects and temporal dynamics (Hadash et al. 2023).
The literature states that practising mindfulness helps improve reflection and understanding of emotions and mental events and leads to a higher capacity for objectivity regarding personal experiences (Eidman et al. 2022). Additionally, research suggests that meditation influences peoples' morality (Feruglio et al. 2022; Paruzel-Czachura & Kocur 2023), though with mixed results (Du et al. 2023; Reynolds et al. 2023).
Despite the extensive research on mindfulness, some important questions remain unanswered (Jankowski & Bąk, 2019; Karl & Fischer 2022). For example, how does mindfulness mediate the relationship between statistics anxiety and academic dishonesty? The present research elucidates this issue based on prospect theory (Kahneman & Tversky 1979, 2013; Salgado et al. 2022).
Prospect theory: risk aversion and risk seekers
The prospect theory implies that an individual’s framing of a situation determines the risk-taking mindset (Kahneman & Tversky 1979). Risk-seeking (risk-taking or tolerant) behaviours, such as academic dishonesty, involve a potential chance for harm or danger (Lejuez et al. 2002), while risk-averse (or risk-avoidance) behaviours, such as academic integrity, involve avoiding harm or danger. The prospect theory describes the expected utility of a decision under risk (Levy 1992) by positing that if individuals evaluate the outcome as a higher utility than loss, they behave as risk-taking; however, if they consider it a loss, they behave as risk-averse (Liu 2022).
Consideration of risk presumes knowledge of probabilities of outcomes, whereas uncertainty does not. Decision-making often involves working under uncertainty (Vinod 2023). Thus, students’ behavioural decisions, like academic dishonesty, imply risk and uncertainty. If students expect a positive outcome, they may behave with risk aversion, while students who expect negative outcomes may behave as risk seekers (Salgado et al. 2022). Thus, there is a need to study further students’ appetites for risk and academic misconduct decision-making (Birks et al. 2020), and address how to support ethical decision-making in and beyond the classroom (Eaton 2023).
Likewise, mindfulness affects decision-making through decision-framing, according to the way choices are presented or perceived. In other words, mindfulness affects decision-making through decision-framing by inhibitory impulsivity and stress (Gonçalves et al. 2023), promoting awareness, reducing reactivity, and encouraging a focus on the present. Research suggests that mindfulness can potentially mitigate risk since the causal connections are mediated by the attention and present-focus components of mindfulness (Y. Zhang et al. 2021a, b).
The literature has found that mindfulness training reduces risk-taking behaviours (Baltruschat et al. 2021; Upton & Renshaw 2019) and reduces stress (Alem et al. 2021). Furthermore, risk-averse individuals were found to have higher levels of mindfulness than risk seekers (Aumeboonsuke & Caplanova 2021; Y. Zhang et al. 2021a, b). Based on the above, we posit the following:
-
H1: Risk-averse students will be more mindful than risk seekers.
-
Academic integrity—academic dishonesty
Education has been constantly subjected to academic integrity issues (Eshet & Margaliot 2022; Eshet et al. 2022; Steinberger et al. 2021). Academic integrity is essential for education (Ozoliņa & Bēriņa, 2021). It implies fair, responsible, and trustful behaviours (Sefcik et al. 2020). Academic integrity infringements—academic dishonesty—include misconducts like fraudulent behaviour, cheating, or plagiarising (Eshet et al. 2023; Pan et al. 2019). This unethical conduct harms the quality of higher education (Cuadrado et al. 2019; Lee et al. 2020). Furthermore, research points to the quotidian of academic dishonesty behaviour (Krou et al. 2021), and students report engaging in it when needed (Peled et al. 2019).
Engaging in misconduct is perceived as risky due to the uncertainty of negative consequences while receiving a low grade for inadequate work is seen as a more foreseeable outcome. Thus, based on prospect theory (Kahneman & Tversky 1979, 2013; Tversky & Kahneman 1992), the literature affirms that students’ risk and uncertainty decision behaviour (whether academic integrity or academic dishonesty) is related to the expected outcome (Salgado et al. 2022). If students understand that the amount of learning activities required to succeed in a course is higher than the requested activities to pass a course or an exam, they are expected to risk avoidance and act honestly. Nevertheless, they would be more prone to risk-taking and academic dishonesty if expected to fail. Thus, we posit:
-
H2: Risk-averse students’ academic dishonesty will be lower than risk seekers.
Statistics anxiety
Statistical knowledge facilitates the understanding, interpretation, and evaluation of information (e.g., weather probabilities, election results, and physical and mental health decisions) (Trassi et al. 2022). Consequently, in the last decade, statistical literacy has become a compulsory course in interdisciplinary academic education (J. W.Zhang et al. 2021a, b), as well as in humanities and social sciences (Trassi et al. 2022). Several students associate statistical courses with high anxiety levels (O’Bryant et al. 2021) and experience statistics anxiety.
Statistics anxiety indicates a negative emotional belief, attitude, or state incited by any contact with statistically linked content (O’Bryant et al. 2021). The widely acknowledged approach to statistics anxiety (Levpušček & Cukon 2020) is the multi-faceted model as exemplified by the Statistics Anxiety Rating Scale (STARS) (Cruise et al. 1985). The STARS comprehends six components of anxious feelings and learners' attitudes towards statistics: (a) interpretation anxiety—the anxiety triggered by the need to interpret different statistical data; (b) test and class anxiety—the anxiety demonstrated while attending statistics courses and taking statistics tests; (c) fear of asking for help—the anxiety manifested when asking assistance to understand statistics; (d) computational self-concept—a student's perception of his mathematical abilities required for learning statistics; (e) worth of statistics—the significance and relevance of learning statistics to daily or future practise or profession; and (f) fear of statistics teachers—students' perceptions of statisticians and statistics teachers.
Research has observed that statistics anxiety and academic performance are related (Siew et al. 2019; Steinberger et al. 2021) and that statistics anxiety impairs learning (Eshet et al. 2022). Furthermore, negative emotions, like anxiety, influence students' tendency to engage in unethical conduct (Tindall et al. 2021; Zhang et al. 2020). Despite the vast literature on statistics anxiety (Cui et al. 2019; Trassi et al. 2022; Zahan et al. 2020), to our knowledge, none has analysed it under the prospect theory lens. On the one hand, research (Luhmann et al. 2011) has shown that increased uncertainty results in riskier decision-making patterns among anxious individuals. Therefore, it is conceivable that anxious students might demonstrate a higher propensity for risk-taking when confronted with uncertainty about their grades in a statistics exam. On the other hand, other research suggests an association between anxiety and low risk/reward (Prietzel 2020). Individuals with high anxiety levels have overly anxious reactions to risk and exhibit risk-averse behaviour (Shou et al. 2022). Thus, given the diverse outcomes regarding the relationship between anxiety and risk aversion, we posit:
-
H3: Risk-averse students will have higher levels of statistics anxiety than risk seekers.
Academic dishonesty and mindfulness
As previously stated, there is a myriad of literature on academic integrity and dishonesty (Eshet et al. 2023; Tatum 2022). Whereas academic integrity is the pillar of higher education principles of truth (Poitras Pratt & Gladue 2022), academic dishonesty often references various academic integrity violations like contract cheating, plagiarism, and cheating. (Hughes & Eaton 2022). Research has suggested a positive relationship between academic integrity and mindfulness (De Maio & Dixon 2022; Lau 2021). Furthermore, the literature argues that mindfulness restrains unethical behaviour (Y. Zhang et al. 2021a, b). Moreover, studies associating mindfulness with ethical decision-making suggest that individuals engaging in mindfulness practices typically cultivate an enhanced awareness of moral principles and cultural norms. This heightened awareness reduces their inclination to engage in actions that contravene these ethical standards (Orazi et al. 2021). This effect is likely attributable to the way mindfulness shapes ethical and moral choices by fostering self-awareness, emotional regulation, empathy, diminished reactivity, cognitive flexibility, and a sense of integrity. These factors collectively contribute to a more deliberate, compassionate, and morally informed decision-making process, shaping positive ethical decision-making (Culiberg & Mihelič, 2020). Thus, based on the literature, we posit:
-
H4: Mindfulness will negatively impact academic dishonesty.
Mindfulness, academic dishonesty and statistics anxiety
As previously stated, mindfulness is characterized as a psychological trait, a practice (e.g., cultivating mindfulness through meditation), a state of awareness, or a psychological process. Mindfulness elements, such as awareness and nonjudgmental acceptance of one's moment-to-moment experience, are considered potential antidotes to common psychological distress, such as anxiety (Keng et al. 2011). Mindfulness training is acknowledged as cognitive training, as it prompts individuals to comprehend the interplay between their thoughts, emotions, and anxiety-related behaviours (Reangsing et al. 2023). Through this practice, individuals gain heightened awareness and the ability to self-regulate their thoughts and emotions (Dark-Freudeman et al. 2022).
Higher education professionals and researchers have exponentially increased their interest in mindfulness (Ergas & Hadar 2021). Still, research linking academic misconduct, statistics anxiety, and mindfulness is scarce and addresses this issue separately. On the one hand, the research found a relationship between dishonest behaviour and statistics anxiety (Eshet et al. 2021, 2022; Steinberger et al. 2021). On the other hand, mindfulness is negatively related to academic dishonesty (Culiberg & Mihelič, 2020; De Maio & Dixon 2022; Lau 2021). Thus, based on the above, we posit:
-
H5: Mindfulness will mediate the relationship between statistics anxiety and academic dishonesty.
Risk aversion, mindfulness and statistics anxiety
Previous research showed that risk-averse persons report lower anxiety levels (McCleskey & Gruda 2021). Additionally, the literature indicates an association between low risk/reward and anxiety (Prietzel 2020). High-anxiety individuals are prone to risk-averse behaviour (Shou et al. 2022). Furthermore, studies suggested that mindfulness reduces risk-taking behaviours (Aumeboonsuke & Caplanova 2021; Baltruschat et al. 2021; Upton & Renshaw 2019), with anxiety being related to low-risk (Prietzel 2020) and risk-averse behaviour (Shou et al. 2022). Higher education students face several sources of stress (Amanvermez et al. 2022), like statistics anxiety. Thus, based on previous literature on the relationship between anxiety and risk aversion, anxiety and mindfulness and the above, we posit:
-
H6: Risk-aversion will moderate the relationship between statistics anxiety and mindfulness.
Research model
Based on the literature above, the hypothetical structural research model is presented in Fig. 1. The research model presents mindfulness as a mediator between statistics anxiety (measured by worth of statistics, interpretation anxiety, test and class anxiety, computational self-concept, fear of asking for help, and fear of statistics teachers) and academic dishonesty, while the relationship between statistics anxiety and mindfulness is moderated by risk aversion.
Materials and methods
Sample and procedure
A sample size of 791, which was sufficient to detect a Large-sized (d = 0.25, α = 0.05, power = 0.80; G-Power Analysis). Data were collected from undergraduate students in six Israeli academic institutions studying for bachelor's degrees in social sciences. There were 791 participants, 14% of whom were male and 86% of whom were female students. The participants' average age was 25.32 years, ranging from 18 to 60 (SD = 7.32). The average time for filling out the questionnaires was 12 min. In this study, we used the convenience sampling method. About 12% of the participants were excluded from the analysis because their survey instruments were incomplete or carelessly completed (less than 80% in general).
Additional socio-demographic variables were examined: matriculation grade in mathematics (M = 86.44, SD = 11.17), grade point average (M = 86.80, SD = 8.90), Psychometric grade (M = 528.86, SD = 41.79).
Instruments
Risk aversion
Risk Aversion was measured according to the prospect theory framework by Kahneman and Tversky (1979, 2013). Prospect theory is one of the most influential frameworks in behavioural science, particularly in research on decision-making under risk (Ruggeri et al. 2020). The participants were asked to choose between two scenarios involving a 50% chance of gaining $450 and an equal chance of gaining nothing on a coin flip. The results were then scored with 0 for risk-seeking behaviour and 1 for risk aversion.
Mindfulness
The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (Brown and Ryan 2003a, 2003b) is a validated (González-Blanch et al., 2022) and widely used self-report questionnaire that measures sustained attention capacity (Hadash et al. 2023; Isbel et al. 2020; Vaughn et al. 2013). It also measures the disposition to be attentive and aware of the experience of the present moment in daily life (Eidman et al. 2022). The questionnaire consists of 15 items on a 6-point Likert scale (with 1- "Almost never" and 6—"Almost Always"). The total scores were averaged, with a higher score indicating a higher level of mindfulness. The alpha Cronbach of the original questionnaire was 0.87 and 0.82 in the current study, with confidence intervals of 0.21—0.25.
Statistics anxiety
We used the Statistical Anxiety Rating Scale- Hebrew version (Steinberger 2020)—an abridged version of the STARS (Cruise et al. 1985)—internal consistency reliability (0.80–0.94). Previous research has established the confirmatory factor analysis (DeVaney 2016). Steinberger (2020) reported Internal consistency reliability coefficients and descriptive statistics of the Hebrew adaptation of the STARS (N = 163). The 35-item STARS- Hebrew includes six subscales: (a) worth of statistics, (b) interpretation anxiety, (c) test and class anxiety, (d) computational self-concept, (e) fear of asking for help, and (f) fear of statistics teachers. We used a 5-point scale where 1 indicates no anxiety and 5 indicates great anxiety. The total scores were averaged, with a higher score indicating a higher level of statistic anxiety. Steinberger (2020) conducted an exploratory factor analysis to determine the factor structure of statistics anxiety. SA was conceived as multidimensional with six different dimensions. All the loading between the six dimensions was found to be high and significant, ranging from 0.41 to 0.93. The internal consistency reliability of the questionnaire in the current study ranged from 0.77 to 0.89, with confidence intervals ranging from 0.74—0.79 to 0.88 -0. 90. SA was conceived as multidimensional with six dimensions. All the loading between the six dimensions was found to be high and significant, ranging from 0.41 to 0.93.
Academic dishonesty
Academic dishonesty was measured directly using the Academic Misconduct Scale (Bolin 2004)—reliability of 0.91 Cronbach's alpha—and indirectly using the Academic Integrity Inventory (Kisamore et al. 2007)—reliability of 0.75 Cronbach's alpha. Questionnaires were adapted and validated to the Israeli context. Peled et al. (2019) conducted an exploratory factor analysis using the varimax rotation to determine the factor structure of academic dishonesty. Academic dishonesty was conceived as multidimensional, with the two different dimensions with sums of squared loadings ranging from 0.63 to 0.86. We used a 5-point scale where 1 corresponded to "Very unlikely" and 5 to "Very likely". The total scores were averaged, with a higher score indicating a higher level of academic dishonesty. The internal consistency reliability of the questionnaire in the current study was 0.94 for the Academic Misconduct Scale with a confidence interval of 0.93—0.94 and 0.68 for the Academic Integrity Inventory with a confidence interval of 0.64—0.77.
Socio-demographic variables
The questionnaire contained items related to participants’ sociodemographic contexts: age, gender, grade point average, matriculation grade in mathematics, and Psychometric grade.
Plan of analysis
We analysed the data using Structural Equation Modelling (SEM), and full information maximum likelihood estimates were computed using the Analysis of Moment Structures (AMOS) program. The model was examined for the goodness of fit using χ2, comparative fit index (CFI) and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) fit indices. CFI values above 0.90 and 0.95 indicate adequate and good model fit, respectively, and RMSEA values below 0.08 and 0.05 indicate adequate and good model fit, respectively (Hu and t., & Bentler, P. M. , 1999; Kline 2023). Standardised indirect effects were computed for each of the 5,000 bootstrapped samples, and the 95% confidence interval was computed by determining the indirect effects at the 2.5th and 97.5th percentiles.
Additional statistical analyses were used in this research: descriptive measures (M; SD), Pearson correlational analysis, t-tests, and Cronbach’s alpha internal reliability analysis. All the hypotheses were tested at the p < 0.05 level of confidence.
Ethical considerations of the study
The ethics committee approved the study (Reference number 2021/20, approval date: 02/04/2021). Informed consent was collected before enrolment.
Results
This research involved 791 participants, with 12% indicating high levels of statistics anxiety (scoring above 4 on a scale ranging from 1 to 5). Additionally, 65% of respondents acknowledged breaching academic integrity at least once.
Table 1 presents descriptive statistics on academic misconduct. Most of the respondents (55%) admitted to copying sentences without giving credit to the author. Some respondents (39%) acknowledged copying material from fellow students and submitting it as their own. Some respondents reported that they helped someone else cheat on a test (41%).
Table 2 presents descriptive statistics on academic integrity. Most of the respondents (74%) agreed that faculty is very much concerned about academic integrity. Additionally, most students (70%) understand academic integrity requirements.
Table 3 presents Pearson correlations between mindfulness, statistics anxiety, and the dependent variables of academic integrity and misconduct.
The results show significant negative correlations between mindfulness and all the six components of statistics anxiety and between mindfulness and the dependent variables of academic dishonesty: Academic misconduct and academic integrity. The higher the level of a student's mindfulness, the less they will experience statistics anxiety and the less they will engage in acts of academic dishonesty. In addition, there are significant positive correlations between the components of statistics anxiety and the dependent variables of academic dishonesty. The higher the level of a student's level of statistics anxiety, the higher the tendency they will have to behave unethically in academic settings.
Table 4 presents the differences between risk seekers and risk-averse students in statistics anxiety, mindfulness, and academic dishonesty.
As shown in Table 4, there are significant differences in all three research variables, thus confirming H1, H2, and H3. Risk aversion is significantly higher than risk seekers in statistics anxiety and mindfulness but significantly lower in academic dishonesty.
The academic dishonesty variable was modelled by the variables of academic misconduct and academic integrity and by the latent variable of statistics anxiety (measured worth of statistics, interpretation anxiety, test and class anxiety, computational self-concept, fear of asking for help, fear of statistics teachers) with a mediation of mindfulness moderated by risk aversion. The data fit the academic dishonesty model well (χ2 = 228.392, N = 791, df = 27, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.964, RMSEA = 0.097).
Academic misconduct analysis
The structural model of academic misconduct is illustrated in Fig. 2.
The results of the analysis indicate that the variance in academic dishonesty is explained by students' statistics anxiety with a mediation of mindfulness moderated by risk aversion.
As shown in Fig. 2, mindfulness significantly negatively affects academic dishonesty (b = -0.59, p < 0.001). The more mindful the individual is, the less likely they are to engage in unethical behaviours in academic settings. The results provide support for the proposed H4.
Statistic anxiety has no significant direct effect on academic dishonesty (b = 0.04, p = 0.328). However, a significant indirect effect was found between statistic anxiety on academic dishonesty, indicating that mindfulness mediates the relationship. The bootstrapped standardised, indirect effect of mindfulness as a mediator between students' statistic anxiety and academic dishonesty was -0.454 (p < 0.05), and the 95% confidence interval ranged from -1.726 to -0.166. Thus, this indirect effect was statistically significant and support for H5 was obtained.
In addition, risk aversion has a significant direct effect on mindfulness (b = 0.16, p < 0.001), which means that risk-averse individuals tend to have higher levels of mindfulness than risk seekers. Moreover, risk aversion significantly moderates the relationship between statistics anxiety and mindfulness (b = 0.56, p < 0.001), confirming H6.
Figure 3 presents the relationship between statistics anxiety and mindfulness moderated by risk aversion.
As shown in Fig. 3, within individuals with low statistics anxiety, there is no significant difference in the level of mindfulness between risk averse and risk seekers at the specified p < 0.05 level, t(393) = 0.741, p = 0.459, d = 0.79, 95% CI [-0.31, 0.14]. However, among individuals with high statistics anxiety, there is a significant difference in the level of mindfulness between risk averse and risk seekers at the specified p < 0.05 level, t(394) = 2.910, p < 0.01, d = 0.80, 95% CI [-0.58, -0.11], with higher mindfulness among risk averse individuals (M = 4.09, SD = 0.82) than among risk seekers (M = 3.81, SD = 0.74).
Discussion
The present research aim is to answer the following question: How does mindfulness mediate the relationship between statistics anxiety and academic dishonesty, moderated by risk aversion? First, we analysed the differences between risk-seeking and risk-averse students in (a) mindfulness (H1), (b) academic ethical behaviour (H2), and (c) statistics anxiety (H3). Our results show that risk-averse students have higher levels of mindfulness and lower levels of academic dishonesty than risk-seeker students, thus confirming H1 and H2. These results may be due to the positive influence of mindfulness on academic performance (Verhaeghen 2023), utilitarian moral judgment (Paruzel-Czachura & Kocur 2023), risk perception reasoning style (Siegrist & Árvai 2020), and negative effects suppressor (Aksen et al. 2023). At the same time, anxiety influences decision-making by regulating risk behaviour (Y. Zhang et al. 2021a, b). Previous research points out that mindfulness reduces rumination, worry, and fear of future scenarios (Galles et al. 2019).
Second, we examined the relationship between mindfulness and academic dishonesty (H4). The results, in line with previous literature (De Maio & Dixon 2022; Lau 2021), confirm that mindfulness negatively correlates to academic dishonesty. This result may be due to mindfulness’ implicit characteristics as a (un)ethical behaviour regulator (Paruzel-Czachura & Kocur 2023; Y. Zhang et al. 2021a, b).
Third, we analysed how mindfulness mediates the relationship between statistics anxiety and academic dishonesty (H5). The findings indicate a significant indirect effect between statistic anxiety and academic dishonesty, indicating that mindfulness mediates the relationship. The higher the level of a student's statistics anxiety, the lower the mindfulness and, as a result, the higher the tendency to commit academic dishonesty. One possible explanation for how mindfulness might mediate the relationship between statistics anxiety and academic dishonesty is that students high in mindfulness have better emotion regulation skills, such as the ability to recognise and reduce the distraction caused by attending to afflictions (Smith et al. 2023), thus counteracting anxiety and reducing the likelihood of engaging in risky, unethical behaviour, like academic dishonesty. On the contrary, the influx of anxiety feelings obstructs mindfulness capability. The entire mind is distracted by restlessness, worries, and fears, increasing the inability to concentrate and the cheating tendency.
Another explanation may be that, on the one hand, students interpret risk behaviour differently (Irwin et al. 2020). On the other hand, decision-making is linked to awareness situations (Avcu Meriç & Sönmez 2022). Consequently, a student’s mindfulness may help reduce the profit weight in their decision-making (Du et al. 2023) and decrease the reward-seeking behaviour (Reynolds et al. 2023). In other words, a student may be aware of they anxiety levels and react to it, or not, and interact with (un)ethical risky decision-making at the cost of profit or losing it (passing the exam vs. being caught). Paradoxically, a student may behave unethically and neutralize this negative feeling with mindfulness by not judging the unwanted behaviour.
Finally, results confirmed the moderating effect of risk aversion on the relationship between statistics anxiety and mindfulness (H6). The findings showed that among students suffering from high statistics anxiety, there is a significant difference in the level of mindfulness between risk-averse and risk-seekers. No such difference was found among students with low statistics anxiety. This finding confirms that risk aversion moderates the effect of statistics anxiety on mindfulness. Students with high statistics anxiety levels are prone to risk-seeking behaviours, experience lower mindfulness, and are inclined to commit academic dishonesty. This outcome can be explained by the assumption that these risk-taker students are motivated by the potential rewards and make controverted decisions without fully considering the consequences of their actions (Kurdoglu et al. 2023). In other words, risk is a forward-looking idea that captures the eventuality of an occurrence (Vinod 2023). The risk-taker consciousness centres on the distant future, its outcomes, and possible profits and benefits—contrasting mindfulness, whose focus seeks to be here and now, accepting the circumstances of the moment. In addition, statistics anxiety can increase stress levels, impair cognitive functioning and decision-making abilities (Browning et al. 2015; Hartley & Phelps 2012), and decrease mindfulness, thus influencing the likelihood of engaging in academic dishonesty.
In contrast, anxious, risk-averse counterparts tend to be more mindful of their situation and behaviour and less frequently engage in academic misconduct. Risk aversion is avoiding risks that may lead to negative consequences (Hartley & Phelps 2012). Risk-averse students are more cautious and may approach a challenging situation with greater awareness and a greater sense of control, which reduces the likelihood of engaging in academically unethical behaviours. These results align with previous studies examining the relationship between mindfulness and risk aversion (Baltruschat et al. 2021).
Conclusion and practical implications
In line with the literature, students’ academic misconduct decision-making may be changed (Draper et al. 2021). Thus, we suggest that mindfulness-based interventions might be a constructive tool to reduce risk-taking and promote ethical decision-making among individuals who experience high levels of statistics anxiety. Furthermore, developing these skills may succour individuals with higher anxiety levels, guide them on managing it, and help them avoid engaging in academically unethical behaviours.
Mindfulness is a universal acquirable skill available to everyone (Morgan & Katz 2021). Therefore, based on the current research results, we recommend implementing mindfulness training in academic institutions to reduce students' anxieties (David et al. 2022), specifically in compulsory statistics courses. Through consistent and continuous practice, the students will acquire tools to deal with their anxieties and positively affect avoidance and unethical behaviour. Thus, when the level of anxiety about statistics rises (toward the date of an exam or a deadline for a difficult assignment), students will have the skill to express anxiety without dramatizing it.
Moreover, these proactive interventions enhance academic integrity behaviour. By being nonjudgmental and fully present in the moment, individuals can contemplate their thoughts and emotions without becoming overwhelmed, thereby allowing them to respond ethically. For example, if a student is experiencing statistics anxiety and is tempted to cheat on a test, mindfulness might help them recognize the temptation as a passing thought rather than acting on it. By being fully present and nonjudgmental, students can take a deep breath, refocus their attention, and tackle the task with a clear mind and ethical intention.
Limitations and future research
This research also has limitations. First, due to the gap in the literature on the relationship between the research variables, this is exploratory research. Furthermore, data was collected using a subjective self-report questionnaire. Future research may benefit from dyadic data, like self-report and objective data, with plagiarism detection software. Furthermore, exploring the impact of experimental mindfulness interventions on academic dishonesty and statistics anxiety could be a promising avenue for future research. Additionally, future research may compare and measure risk preferences according to the multidimensional DOSPERT (Blais & Weber 2006), a psychometrically valid measure.
Additionally, our samples were not gender balanced. Thus, we recommend conducting future studies on gender comparisons. Finally, our research was conducted in only one culturally diverse Westernised country, so future studies should include samples from other countries with different backgrounds.
Availability of data and materials
Data and materials would be sent upon request.
Abbreviations
- SEM:
-
Structural Equation Modelling
- STARS:
-
Statistics Anxiety Rating Scale
- MAAS:
-
Mindful Attention Awareness Scale
- AMOS:
-
Analysis of Moment Structures
- CFI:
-
Comparative Fit Index
- RMSEA:
-
Root Mean Square Error Of Approximation
References
Aksen, D. E., Sleight, F. G., & Lynn, S. J. (2023). Mindfulness intervention for impulsivity as a stand-alone treatment: A qualitative review of emerging evidence. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000367
Alem Y, Behrendt H, Belot M, Bíró A (2021) Mind training, stress and behaviour—A randomised experiment. PLoS ONE 16(11):e0258172. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258172
Amanvermez, Y., Zhao, R., Cuijpers, P., de Wit, L. M., Ebert, D. D., Kessler, R. C., Bruffaerts, R., & Karyotaki, E. (2022). Effects of self-guided stress management interventions in college students: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Internet Interventions, 28, 100503. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.invent.2022.100503
Aumeboonsuke V, Caplanova A (2021) An analysis of impact of personality traits and mindfulness on risk aversion of individual investors. Curr Psychol 42(8):1–18. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02015-9
Avcu Meriç I, Sönmez MB (2022) Decision-making, interoceptive awareness and mindful attention awareness in male patients with alcohol use disorder. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 27(1):35–48. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546805.2021.2011183
Baltruschat S, Mas-Cuesta L, Cándido A, Maldonado A, Verdejo-Lucas C, Catena-Verdejo E, Catena A (2021) Repeat traffic offenders improve their performance in risky driving situations and have fewer accidents following a mindfulness-based intervention. Front Psychol 11:4012–4012. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2020.567278/BIBTEX
Birks M, Mills J, Allen S, Tee S (2020) Managing the mutations: Academic misconduct Australia, New Zealand, and the UK. Int J Educ Integr 16(6):1–15. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40979-020-00055-5
Blais A-R, Weber EU (2006) A domain-specific risk-taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgm Decis Mak 1(1):33–47. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500000334
Bolin AU (2004) Self-control, perceived opportunity, and attitudes as predictors of academic dishonesty. J Psychol 138(2):101–114. https://doi.org/10.3200/JRLP.138.2.101-114
Brown KW, Ryan RM (2003a) The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol 84(4):822–822. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
Brown KW, Ryan RM (2003b) Mindful attention awareness scale. J Pers Soc Psychol
Browning M, Behrens TE, Jocham G, O’Reilly JX, Bishop SJ (2015) Anxious individuals have difficulty learning the causal statistics of aversive environments. Nat Neurosci 18(4):590–596. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3961
Cruise, R. J., Cash, R. W., & Bolton, D. L. (1985). Development and validation of an instrument to measure statistical anxiety. American Statistical Association Proceedings of the Section on Statistical Education,
Cuadrado D, Salgado JF, Moscoso S (2019) Prevalence and correlates of academic dishonesty: Towards a sustainable university. Sustainability 11(21):6062–6062. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11216062
Cuadrado D, Salgado JF, Moscoso S (2020) Personality, intelligence, and counterproductive academic behaviors: A meta-analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol 120(2):504–537. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000285
Cui S, Zhang J, Guan D, Zhao X, Si J (2019) Antecedents of statistics anxiety: An integrated account. Personality Individ Differ 144:79–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.02.036
Culiberg B, Mihelič KK (2020) The impact of mindfulness and perceived importance of peer reporting on students’ response to peers’ academic dishonesty. Ethics Behav 30(5):385–399. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508422.2019.1628644
Dark-Freudeman A, Jones C, Terry C (2022) Mindfulness, anxiety, and perceived stress in university students: Comparing a mindfulness-based intervention (MBI) against active and traditional control conditions. J Am Coll Health 70(7):2116–2125. https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2020.1845180
David A, Rubinsten O, Berkovich-Ohana A (2022) Math anxiety, self-centeredness, and dispositional mindfulness. J Educ Psychol 114(2):393. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000550
Dawson G (2021) Zen and the mindfulness industry. The Humanistic Psychologist 49(1):133–146. https://doi.org/10.1037/hum000017
De Maio C, Dixon K (2022) Promoting academic integrity in institutions of higher learning: What 30 years of research (1990–2020) in Australasia has taught us. Journal of College and Character 23(1):6–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/2194587X.2021.2017972
DeVaney TA (2016) Confirmatory factor analysis of the statistical anxiety rating scale with online graduate students. Psychol Rep 118(2):565–586. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294116644093
Draper M, Lancaster T, Dann S, Crockett R, Glendinning I (2021) Essay mills and other contract cheating services: to buy or not to buy and the consequences of students changing their minds. Int J Educ Integr 17(1):13. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40979-021-00081-x
Du W, Yu H, Liu X, Zhou X (2023) Mindfulness training reduces slippery slope effects in moral decision-making and moral judgment. Sci Rep 13(1):2967. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-29614-9
Eaton SE (2023) Postplagiarism: transdisciplinary ethics and integrity in the age of artificial intelligence and neurotechnology. Int J Educ Integr 19(1):23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40979-023-00144-1
Eaton, S. E. (2021). Plagiarism in higher education: Tackling tough topics in academic integrity. ABC-CLIO.
Eidman, L., Rodriguez de Behrends, M., & Seif, G. (2022). Psychometric properties of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) in Argentine university students. Ciencias Psicológicas, 16(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.22235/cp.v16i1.2560
Eltayeb, F. (2021). Mindfulness and its relation to academic procrastination among university students. Universal Journal of Educational Research, 9(5), 917–927. https://doi.org/10.13189/ujer.2021.090504
Ergas, O., & Hadar, L. L. (2021). Does mindfulness belong in higher education? An eight year research of students’ experiences. Pedagogy, Culture & Society, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/14681366.2021.1906307
Eshet Y, Steinberger P, Grinautsky K (2022) Does statistics anxiety impact academic dishonesty? Academic challenges in the age of distance learning. Int J Educ Integr 18(1):1–19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40979-022-00117-w
Eshet Y, Dickman N, Ben Zion Y (2023) Academic integrity in the HyFlex learning environment. Heliyon 9(2):e13301. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2023.e13301
Eshet, Y., & Margaliot, A. (2022). Does creative thinking contribute to the academic integrity of education students? Frontiers in Psychology, 4709–4709. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2022.925195
Eshet, Y., Steinberger, P., & Grinautsky, K. (2021). Relationship between statistics anxiety and academic dishonesty: A comparison between learning environments in social sciences. Sustainability, 13(3), 1–18–18. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13031564
Feruglio S, Panasiti MS, Crescentini C, Aglioti SM, Ponsi G (2022) The impact of mindfulness meditation on social and moral behavior: Does mindfulness enhance other-oriented motivation or decrease monetary reward salience? Front Integr Neurosci 16:963422. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2022.963422
Galles J, Lenz J, Peterson GW, Sampson JP Jr (2019) Mindfulness and decision-making style: Predicting career thoughts and vocational identity. The Career Development Quarterly 67(1):77–91. https://doi.org/10.1002/cdq.12164
Gonçalves AP, Donate M, E. C. d., Bedendo, A., Félix Júnior, I., Gallo, G. G., & Opaleye, E. S. (2023) The effect of mindfulness on decision-making, inhibitory control, and impulsivity of substance use disorder in-treatment patients: A randomized clinical trial. PLoS ONE 18(11):e0293502–e0293502. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293502
González-Blanch C, Medrano LA, O'Sullivan S, Bell I, Nicholas J, Chambers R, ... Alvarez-Jimenez M (2022) Psychometric properties of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) in a first-episode psychosis sample. Psychol Assess 34(2):188
Götmann, A., Bechtoldt, M. N., & Fetchenhauer, D. (2021). Breathing to the path of virtue - The effects of justice sensitivity and state mindfulness on cheating behavior in a group performance task. Personality and Individual Differences, 178(March). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.110851
Hadash Y, Ruimi L, Bernstein A (2023) Looking inside the black box of mindfulness meditation: Investigating attention and awareness during meditation using the mindful awareness task (MAT). Psychol Assess 35(3):242. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0001194
Hartley CA, Phelps EA (2012) Anxiety and decision-making. Biol Psychiat 72(2):113–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.027
Hasty LM, Malanchini M, Shakeshaft N, Schofield K, Malanchini M, Wang Z (2021) When anxiety becomes my propeller: Mental toughness moderates the relation between academic anxiety and academic avoidance. Br J Educ Psychol 91(1):368–390. https://doi.org/10.1111/BJEP.12366
Hirshberg MJ, Colaianne BA, Greenberg MT, Inkelas KK, Davidson RJ, Germano D, Dunne JD, Roeser RW (2022) Can the academic and experiential study of flourishing improve flourishing in college students? A Multi-University Study Mindfulness 13(9):2243–2256. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-022-01952-1
Hu L, t., & Bentler, P. M. (1999) Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model 6(1):1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
Hughes JC, Eaton SE (2022) Academic misconduct in Higher Education: Beyond student cheating. In: Eaton SE, Hughes C (eds) Academic Integrity in Canada. Springer, pp 81–102
Irwin A, Sedlar N, Hamlet O (2020) Flying solo: A vignette-based examination of general aviation pilot risk perception and decision-making. Aviation Psychology and Applied Human Factors 10(2):59–69. https://doi.org/10.1027/2192-0923/a000189
Isbel B, Stefanidis K, Summers MJ (2020) Assessing mindfulness: Experimental support for the discriminant validity of breath counting as a measure of mindfulness but not self-report questionnaires. Psychol Assess 32(12):1184. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000957
Jankowski T, Bąk W (2019) Mindfulness as a mediator of the relationship between trait anxiety, attentional control and cognitive failures. A multimodel inference approach. Personality Individ Differ 142:62–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2019.01.034
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–292. https://www.jstor.org/stable/1914185
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (2013). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. In Handbook of the fundamentals of financial decision making: Part I (pp. 99–127). World Scientific.
Karl JA, Fischer R (2022) The state of dispositional mindfulness research. Mindfulness 13(6):1357–1372. https://doi.org/10.1007/S12671-022-01853-3/TABLES/7
Kaufmann L, Ninaus M, Weiss EM, Gruber W, Wood G (2022). Self-Efficacy Matters: Influence of Students’ Perceived Self-Efficacy on Statistics Anxiety. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14797
Keng S-L, Smoski MJ, Robins CJ (2011) Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: A review of empirical studies. Clin Psychol Rev 31(6):1041–1056. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006
Kisamore JL, Stone TH, Jawahar IM (2007) Academic integrity: The relationship between individual and situational factors on misconduct contemplations. J Bus Ethics 75(4):381–394. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-006-9260-9
Kline, R. B. (2023). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (Fifth edition ed.). Guilford New York, NY. https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&scope=site&db=nlebk&db=nlabk&AN=3591678
Krou MR, Fong CJ, Hoff MA (2021) Achievement motivation and academic dishonesty: A meta-analytic investigation. Educ Psychol Rev 33:427–458. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09557-7
Kurdoglu RS, Ates NY, Lerner DA (2023) Decision-making under extreme uncertainty: Eristic rather than heuristic. Int J Entrep Behav Res 29(3):763–782. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJEBR-07-2022-0587
Kuroda Y, Yamakawa O, Ito M (2022) Benefits of mindfulness in academic settings: Trait mindfulness has incremental validity over motivational factors in predicting academic affect, cognition, and behavior. BMC Psychology 10(1):1–14. https://doi.org/10.1186/S40359-022-00746-3/TABLES/2
Lau P (2021) A case study on research postgraduate students’ understanding of academic integrity at a Hong Kong University. Frontiers in Education 6:107–107. https://doi.org/10.3389/FEDUC.2021.647626/XML/NLM
Lee SD, Kuncel NR, Gau J (2020) Personality, attitude, and demographic correlates of Academic Dishonesty: A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 146(11):1042–1058. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000300
Lejuez CW, Richards JB, Read JP, Kahler CW, Ramsey SE, Stuart GL, Strong DR, Brown RA (2002) Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). J Exp Psychol Appl 8(2):75–75. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.8.2.75
Lesser, L. M. (2017). Finding center: Towards increasing clarity and reducing anxiety in statistics class. Journal for the Liberal Arts and Sciences, 22(1), 42–58. https://www.oak.edu/wp-content/uploads/2021/06/jlas-v22-i1_0.pdf#page=42
Levpušček, M. P., & Cukon, M. (2020). That old devil called ‘Statistics’: Statistics Anxiety in University Students and related factors. Center for Educational Policy Studies Journal. https://doi.org/10.26529/cepsj.826
Levy, J. S. (1992). An introduction to prospect theory. Political Psychology, 13(2), 171–186. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3791677
Liu, X. (2022). Essays on Prospect Theory and cost structures Kent State University , College of Business Administration ].
Luhmann CC, Ishida K, Hajcak G (2011) Intolerance of uncertainty and decisions about delayed, probabilistic rewards. Behav Ther 42(3):378–386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2010.09.002
McCleskey, J., & Gruda, D. (2021). Risk-taking, resilience, and state anxiety during the COVID-19 pandemic: A coming of (old) age story. Personality and Individual Differences, 170, 110485. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110485
Morgan, W. J., & Katz, J. (2021). Mindfulness meditation and foreign language classroom anxiety: Findings from a randomized control trial. Foreign Language Annals, 54(2), 389–409. https://doi.org/10.1111/flan.12525
Müller, L. K. (2021). Mindfulness in ethical decision-making: How state and trait mindfulness impact moral judgment and acceptance of unethical behavior Universidade Católica Portuguesa].
O’Bryant M, Natesan Batley P, Onwuegbuzie AJ (2021) Validation of an adapted version of the Statistical Anxiety Scale in English and its relationship to attitudes toward statistics. SAGE Open 11:1–15. https://doi.org/10.1177/21582440211001378
Onwuegbuzie AJ, Da Ros D, Ryan JM (1997) The components of statistics anxiety: A phenomenological study. Focus Learn Probl Math 19(4):11–35
Orazi DC, Chen J, Chan EY (2021) To erect temples to virtue: Effects of state mindfulness on other-focused ethical behaviors. J Bus Ethics 169(4):785–798. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-019-04296-4
Ozoliņa, R., & Bēriņa, L. H. (2021). Academic integrity in Latvia’s higher education institutions. SSE Riga Student Research Papers, 6(238), 1–55. https://www.sseriga.edu/sites/default/files/2021-05/6Paper_Ozolina_Berina.pdf
Pan, M., Stiles, B. L., Tempelmeyer, T. C., & Wong, N. (2019). A cross-cultural exploration of academic dishonesty: Current challenges, preventive measures, and future directions. In Prevention and Detection of Academic Misconduct in Higher Education (pp. 63–82). IGI Global.
Parsons D, Gardner P, Parry S, Smart S (2022) Mindfulness-based approaches for managing stress, anxiety and depression for Health students in Tertiary Education: A scoping review. Mindfulness 13:1–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-021-01740-3
Paruzel-Czachura M, Kocur D (2023) Do meditation, mindfulness, and self-compassion impact utilitarian moral judgment? Mindfulness. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-023-02161-0
Peled, Y., Eshet, Y., Barczyk, C., & Grinautski, K. (2019). Predictors of academic dishonesty among undergraduate students in online and face-to-face courses. Computers & Education, 131, 49–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2018.05.012
Poitras Pratt, Y., & Gladue, K. (2022). Re-defining academic integrity: Embracing indigenous truths. In Academic Integrity in Canada (pp. 103–123). Springer.
Prietzel TT (2020) The effect of emotion on risky decision making in the context of prospect theory: a comprehensive literature review. Management Review Quarterly 70(3):313–353. https://doi.org/10.1007/S11301-019-00169-2/TABLES/7
Reangsing C, Trakooltorwong P, Maneekunwong K, Thepsaw J, Oerther S (2023) Effects of online mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) on anxiety symptoms in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Complementary Medicine and Therapies 23(1):269. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12906-023-04102-9
Reynolds SJ, Eliseo M, Watkins TS, Mariam M (2023) Mindful but forgetful: The negative effect of trait mindfulness on memories of immoral behavior. Bus Soc Rev. https://doi.org/10.1111/basr.12328
Ruggeri, K., Alí, S., Berge, M. L., Bertoldo, G., Bjørndal, L. D., Cortijos-Bernabeu, A., Davison, C., Demić, E., Esteban-Serna, C., Friedemann, M., Gibson, S. P., Jarke, H., Karakasheva, R., Khorrami, P. R., Kveder, J., Andersen, T. L., Lofthus, I. S., McGill, L., Nieto, A. E., . . . Folke, T. (2020). Replicating patterns of prospect theory for decision under risk. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(6), 622–633. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0886-x
Salgado, J. F., Cuadrado, D., & Moscoso, S. (2022). Counterproductive academic behaviors and academic performance: A meta-analysis and a path analysis model. Frontiers in Psychology, 0, 3029–3029. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2022.893775
Sefcik L, Striepe M, Yorke J (2020) Mapping the landscape of academic integrity education programs: What approaches are effective? Assess Eval High Educ 45(1):30–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/02602938.2019.1604942
Shou Y, Olney, & Joel. (2022) Measuring risk tolerance across domains: Scale development and validation. J Pers Assess 104(4):484–495. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2021.1966019
Siegrist M, Árvai J (2020) Risk Perception: Reflections on 40 Years of Research. Risk Anal 40(S1):2191–2206. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.13599
Siew CSQ, McCartney MJ, Vitevitch MS (2019) Using network science to understand statistics anxiety among college students. Scholarsh Teach Learn Psychol 5(1):75–89. https://doi.org/10.1037/stl0000133
Smit B, Stavrulaki E (2021) The efficacy of a mindfulness-based intervention for college students under extremely stressful conditions. Mindfulness 12(12):3086–3100. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-021-01772-9
Smith R, Persich MR, Chuning AE, Cloonan S, Woods-Lubert R, Skalamera J, Berryhill SM, Weihs KL, Lane RD, Allen JJB, Dailey NS, Alkozei A, Vanuk JR, Killgore WDS (2023) Improvements in mindfulness, interoceptive and emotional awareness, emotion regulation, and interpersonal emotion management following completion of an online emotional skills training program. Emotion, No Pagination Specified-No Pagination Specified. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001237
Steinberger P (2020) Assessing the Statistical Anxiety Rating Scale as applied to prospective teachers in an Israeli Teacher-Training College. Stud Educ Eval 64:1–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.stueduc.2019.100829
Steinberger, P., Eshet, Y., & Grinautsky, K. (2021). No anxious student is left behind: Statistics anxiety, personality traits, and academic dishonesty—lessons from covid‐19. Sustainability, 13(9). https://doi.org/10.3390/su13094762
Tatum HE (2022) Honor codes and academic integrity: Three decades of research. Journal of College and Character 23(1):32–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/2194587X.2021.2017977
Tindall IK, Fu KW, Tremayne K, Curtis GJ (2021) Can negative emotions increase students’ plagiarism and cheating? Int J Educ Integr 17(1):1–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40979-021-00093-7
Trassi AP, Leonard SJ, Rodrigues LD, Rodas JA, Santos FH (2022) Mediating factors of statistics anxiety in university students: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ann N Y Acad Sci. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14746
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in Prospect Theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5, 297–323. https://www.jstor.org/stable/41755005
Upton SR, Renshaw TL (2019) Immediate effects of the mindful body scan practice on risk-taking behavior. Mindfulness 10(1):78–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/S12671-018-0948-6/FIGURES/1
Vaughn LA, Dubovi AS-Y, Paul Niño N (2013) Processing fluency affects behavior more strongly among people higher in trait mindfulness. J Res Pers 47(6):782–788. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.08.002
Verhaeghen P (2023) Mindfulness and academic performance meta-analyses on interventions and correlations. Mindfulness. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-023-02138-z
Vinod, T. (2023). Understanding risk. In Risk and Resilience in the Era of Climate Change (pp. 35–52). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-19-8621-5_3
Widhiastuti, R., & Kanaka, S. (2021). Academic procrastination with self-control as moderator - Haifa University. Economic Education Analysis Journal, 10(2), 240–252. https://doi.org/10.15294/eeaj.v10i2.46623
Zahan FN, Islam MA, Kawsar LA (2020) Relationships among statistics anxiety, depression and academic Performance. International Journal of Statistical Sciences 19:35–52. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-16-1919-9
Zhang H, Shi Y, Zhou ZE, Ma H, Tang H (2020) Good people do bad things: How anxiety promotes unethical behavior through intuitive and automatic processing. Curr Psychol 39(2):720–728. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9789-7
Zhang JW, Kessler E, Braasch JLG (2021a) Self-compassion mindsets can predict statistics course performance via intelligence mindsets and statistics anxiety. Learn Individ Differ 90:1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2021.102047
Zhang Y, Chen ZJ, Lu S, Ni S (2021b) Are mindful people more risk-averse? Effects of trait and state mindfulness on risk preference in decision-making. Int J Psychol 56(3):407–414. https://doi.org/10.1002/IJOP.12716
Acknowledgements
We would also like to express our gratitude to Mrs. Ariane Cukierkorn, Information Specialist, for her helpful and constructive comments and suggestions, and for her help proofreading and editing the manuscript.
Funding
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Not applicable.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the. The consent Zefat Academic College was given in written form (number of decision: RDR 15051).
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Eshet, Y., Grinautsky, K. & Steinberger, P. To behave or not (un)ethically? The meditative effect of mindfulness on statistics anxiety and academic dishonesty moderated by risk aversion. Int J Educ Integr 20, 6 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40979-024-00151-w
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40979-024-00151-w